This test can no longer be taken. It is now part of The Marathon Test.
31 | * |
33 | * |
37 | * |
39 | * |
44 | * |
45 | * |
46 | **** |
47 | ** |
48 | ** |
Test name | n | r |
---|---|---|
Cooijmans Intelligence Test - Form 2 | 3 | 1.00 |
Logima Strictica 36 (Robert Lato) | 3 | 0.99 |
Reason | 3 | 0.99 |
Lieshout International Mesospheric Intelligence Test | 3 | 0.97 |
Strict Logic Sequences Exam I (Jonathan Wai) | 7 | 0.82 |
Spatial Insight Test | 7 | 0.75 |
Non-Verbal Cognitive Performance Examination (Xavier Jouve) | 5 | 0.67 |
Qoymans Multiple-Choice #4 | 7 | 0.58 |
The Final Test | 4 | 0.41 |
Sigma Test (Melão Hindemburg) | 3 | 0.19 |
Miscellaneous tests | 6 | 0.10 |
Weighted average of correlations: 0.647 (N = 51)
Estimated g factor loading: 0.80
These are estimated g factor loadings, but against homogeneous tests (containing only particular item types) as opposed to non-compound heterogeneous tests. Although tending to surprise the lay person, it is not uncommon for tests to have high loadings on item types they do not actually contain themselves. Such loadings reflect the empirical fact that most tests for mental abilities measure primarily g, regardless of their contents; that the major part of test score variance is caused by g, and only a minor part by factors germane to particular item types. It is of key importance to understand that this is a fact of nature, a natural phenomenon, and not something that was built into the tests by the test constructors.
Type | n | g loading of Numerical Insight Test on that type |
---|---|---|
Verbal | 11 | 0.72 |
Numerical | 7 | 0.90 |
Spatial | 10 | 0.90 |
Logical | 3 | 1.00 |
Heterogeneous | 11 | 0.79 |
N = 42
Balanced g loading = 0.86
Country | n | median score |
---|---|---|
Germany | 5 | 47.0 |
Correlation of this test with national average I.Q.'s published by Lynn and Vanhanen, later Lynn and Becker:
Personalia | n | r |
---|---|---|
Disorders (parents and siblings) | 13 | 0.24 |
Year of birth | 14 | 0.23 |
Educational level | 13 | 0.14 |
Mother's educational level | 13 | 0.00 |
Father's educational level | 13 | -0.12 |
Disorders (own) | 13 | -0.30 |
In parentheses the number of score pairs on which that estimated g factor loading is based. The goal of this is to verify the hypothesis that g becomes less important, accounts for a smaller proportion of the variance, at higher I.Q. levels. The mere fact of restricting the range like this also depresses the g loading compared to computing it over the test's full range, so it would be normal for these values to be lower than the test's full-range g loading.
Below 1st quartile | -0.45 (10) |
---|---|
Below median | 0.75 (48) |
Above median | 0.16 (36) |
Above 3rd quartile | 0.74 (9) |
Age class | n | Median score |
---|---|---|
55 to 59 | 1 | 33.0 |
45 to 49 | 1 | 47.0 |
40 to 44 | 1 | 39.0 |
35 to 39 | 2 | 46.0 |
30 to 34 | 3 | 46.0 |
25 to 29 | 1 | 31.0 |
22 to 24 | 1 | 48.0 |
20 or 21 | 2 | 41.5 |
17 | 1 | 44.0 |
16 | 1 | 48.0 |
N = 14
Year taken | n | median score | protonorm |
---|---|---|---|
2003 | 4 | 46.0 | 487 |
2004 | 5 | 37.0 | 386 |
2005 | 5 | 46.0 | 487 |
ryear taken × median score = 0.00 (N = 14)
Item statistics are not published as that would help candidates. To detect bad items, answers and comments from candidates are studied, as well as, for each problem, the correlation with total score on the remaining problems (item-rest correlation) and the proportion of candidates getting it wrong (hardness of the item). Possible bad items are revised, replaced, or removed, possibly resulting in a revised version of the test.